Adrian, C., Griffith, S. C., Naguib, M., Schuett, W. 2022. Wild zebra finches are attracted towards acoustic cues from conspecific social groups. Behavioral Ecology 33: 556-564.
Aellen, M., Burkart, J. M., Bshary, R. 2022. No evidence for general intelligence in a fish. Ethology 128: 424-436.
Aellen, M., Dufour, V., Bshary, R. 2021. Cleaner fish and other wrasse match primates in their ability to delay gratification. Animal Behaviour 176: 125-143.
Afseth, C., Shim, A., Anderson, S., Bell, A. M., Hellmann, J. K. 2022. Vertical transmission of horizontally acquired social information in sticklebacks: implications for transgenerational plasticity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 289: 20220571.
Agarwal, A., Sarel, A., Derdikman, D., Ulanovsky, N., Gutfreund, Y. 2023. Spatial coding in the hippocampus and hyperpallium of flying owls. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 120: e2212418120.
Agee, L. A., Monfils, M.-H. 2018. Effect of demonstrator reliability and recency of last demonstration on acquisition of a socially transmitted food preference. Royal Society Open Science 5: 172391.
Agrawal, S., Riffell, Jeffrey A. 2011. Behavioral neurobiology: The bitter life of male flies. Current Biology 21: R470-R472.
Agrillo, C., Bisazza, A. 2018. Understanding the origin of number sense: a review of fish studies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 373: 0511.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Bisazza, A. 2007. Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition 10: 63-70.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G., Bisazza, A. 2008. Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition 11: 495-503.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G., Bisazza, A. 2009. Use of number by fish. PLoS One 4: e4786.
Agrillo, C., Gori, S., Beran, M. J. 2015. Do rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) perceive illusory motion?. Animal Cognition 18: 895-910.
Agrillo, C., Petrazzini, M. E. M., Bisazza, A. 2014. Numerical acuity of fish is improved in the presence of moving targets, but only in the subitizing range. Animal Cognition 17: 307-316.
Agrillo, C., Piffer, L., Bisazza, A., Butterworth, B. 2012. Evidence for two numerical systems that are similar in humans and guppies. PLoS One 7: e31923.
Aguilar-Arguello, S., Nelson, X. J. 2021. Jumping spiders: An exceptional group for comparative cognition studies. Learn Behav 49: 276-291.
Aguilar-Arguello, S., Taylor, A. H., Nelson, X. J. 2021. Jumping spiders attend to information from multiple modalities when preparing to jump. Animal Behaviour 171: 99-109.
Aguilar-Melo, A. R., Calmé, S., Smith-Aguilar, S. E., Ramos-Fernandez, G. 2018. Fission-fusion dynamics as a temporally and spatially flexible behavioral strategy in spider monkeys. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 72: 150.
Aguiñaga, J., Gomulkiewicz, R., Watts, H. E. 2021. Effect of social information on an individual's assessment of its environment. Animal Behaviour 178: 267-277.
Aharon, G., Sadot, M., Yovel, Y. 2017. Bats use path integration rather than acoustic flow to assess flight distance along flyways. Current Biology 27: 3650-3657.e3.
Ahlgren, J., Chapman, B. B., Nilsson, P. A., Brönmark, C. 2015. Individual boldness is linked to protective shell shape in aquatic snails. Biology Letters 11: 20150029.

<<First    < Previous    1 2 3 4 5 6 7     Next >    Last >>