Aarestrup, K., Okland, F., Hansen, M. M., Righton, D., Gargan, P., Castonguay, M., Bernatchez, L., Howey, P., Sparholt, H., Pedersen, M. I., McKinley, R. S. 2009. Oceanic spawning migration of the european eel (Anguilla anguilla). Science 325: 1660.
Abril-de-Abreu, R., Cruz, A. S., Oliveira, R. F. 2015. Social dominance modulates eavesdropping in zebrafish. Royal Society Open Science 2: 150220.
Adriaenssens, B., Johnsson, J. I. 2011. Shy trout grow faster: exploring links between personality and fitness-related traits in the wild. Behavioral Ecology 22: 135-143.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Bisazza, A. 2007. Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition 10: 63-70.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G., Bisazza, A. 2008. Do fish count? Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition 11: 495-503.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G., Bisazza, A. 2009. Use of number by fish. PLoS One 4: e4786.
Agrillo, C., Petrazzini, M. E. M., Bisazza, A. 2014. Numerical acuity of fish is improved in the presence of moving targets, but only in the subitizing range. Animal Cognition 17: 307-316.
Agrillo, C., Piffer, L., Bisazza, A., Butterworth, B. 2012. Evidence for two numerical systems that are similar in humans and guppies. PLoS One 7: e31923.
Ang, T. Z., Manica, A. 2010. Unavoidable limits on group size in a body size-based linear hierarchy. Behavioral Ecology 21: arq062.
Ariyomo, T. O., Watt, P. J. 2013. Aggression and sex differences in lateralization in the zebrafish. Animal Behaviour 86: 617-622.
Armansin, N. C., Lee, K. A., Huveneers, C., Harcourt, R. G. 2016. Integrating social network analysis and fine-scale positioning to characterize the associations of a benthic shark. Animal Behaviour 115: 245-258.
Arnold, C., Taborsky, B. 2010. Social experience in early ontogeny has lasting effects on social skills in cooperatively breeding cichlids. Animal Behaviour 79: 621-630.
Atton, N., Galef, B. J., Hoppitt, W., Webster, M. M., Laland, K. N. 2014. Familiarity affects social network structure and discovery of prey patch locations in foraging stickleback shoals. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 281: 20140579.
Atton, N., Hoppitt, W., Webster, M. M., Galef, B. G., Laland, K. N. 2012. Information flow through threespine stickleback networks without social transmission. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 279: 4272-4278.
Balcombe, J. 2016. Cognitive evidence of fish sentience. Animal Sentience: An Interdisciplinary Journal on Animal Feeling 1: 2.
Balzarini, V., Taborsky, M., Wanner, S., Koch, F., Frommen, J. G. 2014. Mirror, mirror on the wall: the predictive value of mirror tests for measuring aggression in fish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 871-878.
Barbosa, M., Camacho-Cervantes, M., Ojanguren, A. F. 2016. Phenotype matching and early social conditions affect shoaling and exploration decisions. Ethology 122: 171-179.
Bass, A. H., Gilland, E. H., Baker, R. 2008. Evolutionary origins for social vocalization in a vertebrate hindbrain–spinal compartment. Science 321: 417-421.
Beckmann, C., Biro, P. A. 2013. On the validity of a single (boldness) assay in personality research. Ethology 119: 937-947.
Benhaïm, D., Bégout, M.-L., Lucas, G., Chatain, B. 2013. First insight into exploration and cognition in wild caught and domesticated sea bass (Icentrarchus labrax) in a maze. PLoS One 8: e65872.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7     Next >    Last >>