Turner, J. W., LaFleur, R. M., Richardson, A. T., Holekamp, K. E. 2020. Risk-taking in free-living spotted hyenas is associated with anthropogenic disturbance, predicts survivorship, and is consistent across experimental contexts. Ethology 126: 97-110. | |
Turner, J. W., Robitaille, A. L., Bills, P. S., Holekamp, K. E. 2021. Early-life relationships matter: Social position during early life predicts fitness among female spotted hyenas. Journal of Animal Ecology 90: 183-196. | |
Turner-Evans, D. B., Jayaraman, V. 2016. The insect central complex. Current Biology 26: R453-R457. | |
Tuyttens, F. A. M., de Graaf, S., Heerkens, J. L. T., Jacobs, L., Nalon, E., Ott, S., Stadig, L., Van Laer, E., Ampe, B. 2014. Observer bias in animal behaviour research: can we believe what we score, if we score what we believe?. Animal Behaviour 90: 273-280. | |
Tvardíková, K., Fuchs, R. 2010. Tits use amodal completion in predator recognition: a field experiment. Animal Cognition 13: 609-615. | |
Tvardíková, K., Fuchs, R. 2011. Do birds behave according to dynamic risk assessment theory? A feeder experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 727-733. | |
Tworkowski, L., Lesku, J. A. 2019. Behavioural ecology: Sleeping safely carries energetic costs. Current Biology 29: R801-R803. | |
Tyack, P. L. 2020. A taxonomy for vocal learning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 375: 20180406. | |
Tye, K. M., Stuber, G. D., de Ridder, B., Bonci, A., Janak, P. H. 2008. Rapid strengthening of thalamo-amygdala synapses mediates cue-reward learning. Nature 453: 1253-1257. | |
Übernickel, K., Tschapka, M., Kalko, E. K. V. 2013. Selective eavesdropping behaviour in three Neotropical bat species. Ethology 119: 66-76. | |
Uchida, K., Blumstein, D. T. 2021. Habituation or sensitization? Long-term responses of yellow-bellied marmots to human disturbance. Behavioral Ecology 32: 668-678. | |
Udell, M. A. R. 2015. When dogs look back: inhibition of independent problem-solving behaviour in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) compared with wolves (Canis lupus). Biology Letters 11: 20150489. | |
Udell, M. A. R. 2018. A new approach to understanding canine social cognition. Learn Behav 46: 329-330. | |
Udell, M. A. R. 2022. Challenging stereotypes improves understanding of canine behavioral genetics. Learn Behav 50: 441-442. | |
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., Wynne, C. D. L. 2008. Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Animal Behaviour 76: 1767-1773. | |
Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R., Wynne, C. D. L. 2010. The performance of stray dogs (Canis familiaris) living in a shelter on human-guided object-choice tasks. Animal Behaviour 79: 717-725. | |
Udell, M. A. R., Wynne, C. D. L. 2010. Ontogeny and phylogeny: both are essential to human-sensitive behaviour in the genus Canis. Animal Behaviour 79: e9-e14. | |
Udell, M. A., Wynne, C. D. 2011. Reevaluating canine perspective-taking behavior. Learn Behav 39: 318-23. | |
Uematsu, K., Kutsukake, M., Fukatsu, T., Shimada, M., Shibao, H. 2010. Altruistic colony defense by menopausal female insects. Current Biology 20: 1182-1186. | |
Uemura, M., Meglič, A., Zalucki, M. P., Battisti, A., Belušič, G. 2021. Spatial orientation of social caterpillars is influenced by polarized light. Biology Letters 17: 20200736. |
<<First < Previous 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 Next > Last >>